JAM掲載論文検索

Japanese Acupuncture and Moxibustion (Online)

JAM  2010;Vol.6:80-91

A possible double-blind (practitioner-patient masking) acupuncture needle

Takakura Nobuari1,2,3,4), Takayama Miho 1,2,3,4), Kawase Akiko1,2,3,4), Yajima Hiroyoshi 1,2,3,4)
1) Department of Acupuncture and Moxibustion, Faculty of Health Sciences, Tokyo Ariake University of Medical and Health Sciences, Educational Foundation of Hanada Gakuen
2) Second Department of Physiology, Showa University School of Medicine
3) The Foundation for Oriental Medicine Research
4) Hanada college, Japan School of Acupuncture, Moxibustion and Physiotherapy

Abstract

Background: The double-blind (practitioner-patient masking) placebo acupuncture needles have been considered almost impossible to develop until now.
Methods: We designed a double-blind non-penetrating placebo needle, the needle tip of which simply presses against the skin, and a matched penetrating needle. (1) To validate the masking effect for the practitioner, well-experienced 10 acupuncturists each applied 23 non-penetrating needles and 17 penetrating needles to the LI-4 point. After removing each needle, they judged whether the needle was ‘penetrating’, ‘non-penetrating’ or ‘unidentifiable’. (2) An acupuncturist who was pre-informed he will apply penetrating or non-penetrating needle applied penetrating/penetrating needle pairs at bilateral TE-5 points in thirty healthy volunteers. The acupuncturist guessed whether or not the needle was penetrating after removal of each needle. The acupuncturist provided clues to the needle’s authenticity and rated his confidence in identification on a visual analog scale. (3) For the validation of uninformed patient masking, an acupuncturist randomly applied a non-penetrating/penetrating needle pair to the bilateral TE-5 points in 60 subjects. When both applications were completed, we asked them to write down anything that they noticed regarding the needle application and associated sensations. (4) For the validation of informed patient masking, an acupuncturist applied one of the following needle pairs — penetrating/non-penetrating, non-penetrating/non-penetrating, penetrating/penetrating—randomly at bilateral TE-5 points in 114 subjects who were informed that they would receive either a non-penetrating or a penetrating needle. After the application of a pair of needles the subjects reported whether they identified the needle to be non-penetrating or penetrating for each arm.
Results: (1) The mean ± SD of correct/unidentifiable/incorrect answers given by the 10 acupuncturists were 17.0 ± 4.1/6.4 ± 3.6/16.6 ± 3.0, respectively. (2) Of 60 needles, 44 were incorrectly identified by the acupuncturist. Most identifications were made based on the “feeling of needle insertion.” (3) Regarding uninformed patient masking, none of the subjects commented in the questionnaire that they had received a non-penetrating needle. (4) Of the 114 non-penetrating needle applications, the informed subjects identified 64 incorrectly and 50 correctly. Most interestingly, the subjects identified 36 (32%) of 114 penetrating needle applications incorrectly.
Conclusion: These needles have the potential to mask both practitioners and patients from the type of needle used in acupuncture research.

Key words: acupuncture, double blind, placebo

Introduction

The double-blind study design is generally employed as a methodological safeguard to satisfy the current criterion of minimizing bias in medical science (1–5). However, in acupuncture trials the single blind design had been only possible because practitioner masking in acupuncture studies has been considered as almost infeasible (4–11). Therefore, randomized single-blind (patient-masking) placebo controlled studies (12–17) have been acknowledged as the most rigorous methods in clinical acupuncture science. That is, the strongest evidence supporting the efficacy of acupuncture has been obtained using single-blind approaches (12–17) that fail to meet the methodological standards for study blinding in current medical science (1–5).
Several investigators recently invented and validated a patient-masking, single blind needle which to the patient looks and feels like a real needle (8-11). These recently designed sham/placebo devices, which overcome the inadequacies of previous control procedures, provide enhanced evidence in acupuncture studies when it is impossible to blind the practitioner to the intervention (4-8). However, with studies in which only patients are blinded (single-blind studies), the specific effects of acupuncture beyond placebo cannot be fully clarified because the study subjects/patients may have been biased due to presence of unmasked practitioners (1-6, 18-23). As a result, the effectiveness of acupuncture has remained controversial, even though studies of the highest possible quality have been published in leading medical journals (5). Thus, there has been a call for a methodological advance beyond single-blind studies, despite the inherent difficulties in masking the practitioner in acupuncture studies (4-8). Double-blind trials using placebo needles are critically important to ensure that acupuncture research meets the methodological standards of medical science to provide stronger evidence of the effectiveness of acupuncture treatment using needles (1-6). Only then will acupuncture be incorporated into generally accepted practice (1-6, 19-21).
To solve this methodological conundrum of practitioner masking in acupuncture research(4-8), we have designed a double-blind non-penetrating placebo needle and a matched penetrating needle (24-30). Here, we report the design of double-blind (practitioner–patient masking) needles with a statistical evaluation of the masking effect of these needles(24-30).

Methods

Participants
We recruited well-experienced and licensed acupuncturists on the teaching staff and healthy volunteers who were familiar with receiving acupuncture as experimental subjects, and were familiar with the different sensations of needle penetration and de qi from Hanada College. Before the study, the purpose and format were explained and the subjects provided written consent. The Showa University Ethics Committee gave its approval.

Design of double-blind needles (24-30)
We designed a double-blind (practitioner-patient) non-penetrating placebo needle, the tip of which presses against the skin but cannot penetrate it, and a matched penetrating needle with a specified insertion depth to be used in acupuncture research. The non-penetrating needle is identical to the penetrating needle except for being shorter and having a blunt tip. The appearance and feel of the non-penetrating and penetrating needles were indistinguishable from one another (Figure 1).

Validation test 1 for practitioner masking (25, 26)
Ten highly experienced, licensed acupuncture practitioners (mean ± SD age: 41.7 ± 8.8 years; all men) with a mean ± SD duration of acupuncture experience of 12.4 ± 7.8 years participated in this study (Table 1). Forty needles consisted of 23 non-penetrating and 17 penetrating needles (10 mm insertion depth 31) were used. Before the trial began, the practitioners were informed of possible use of both non-penetrating and penetrating needles. Each acupuncturist consecutively applied 40 needles at the LI-4 point 31, using the alternating twirling technique (alternating between rotating the needle clockwise and counterclockwise). Each needle was inserted and pulled out after the stopper had made contact with the top of the guide tube. Immediately after the removal of each needle, the practitioner recorded his judgment of the needle to be ‘penetrating’, ‘non-penetrating’ or ‘unidentifiable’.

Validation test 2 for practitioner masking (27)
We used 30 penetrating/penetrating needle (10 mm insertion depth 31) pairs in this study (Figure 1). However, the subjects and the acupuncturist were informed that the non-penetrating needle would be used in addition to the penetrating needle before the trial to perform this study under double-blind conditions.
An acupuncturist with 28 years of experience applied the pair of needles at bilateral TE-5 points, one needle on each side, in the 30 subjects (31.0 ± 9.8 years; 24 men, 6 women) using the alternating twirling technique. After removal of each needle the acupuncturist recorded whether he judged the needle to be penetrating or non-penetrating. He then reported clues that led to his identification of authenticity of the needle; these were “facial expression,” “body movement,” and “bleeding,” in addition to the “feeling of needle insertion” and “feeling of needle removal.” He also rated his confidence in identification of the needle authenticity (i.e., the degree of certainty about his answer) on a visual analog scale (VAS), the end-points of which were 0 for no confidence and 100 for complete confidence.
We asked the subjects to guess the authenticity of the needle and to rate skin penetration/penetration-like pain and deep dull sensation (de qi), a deep dull pain sensation that is considered essential for a successful acupuncture treatment 31 on a VAS after each removal of the needle. The VAS corresponded to a numerical scale of 0–100, where 0 represented no pain or de qi sensation and 100 the most intense skin penetrating pain or de qi ever experienced during needle insertion.

Validation test 3 for uninformed patient masking (25, 26)
Sixty healthy volunteers (29.7 ± 7.5 years, 35 men, 25 women) were recruited. Before the trial began, the experimental procedure was explained to the subjects as follows: ‘We will apply two needles, which may or may not differ in type, at bilateral TE-5 points (31).
The acupuncturist applied a pair of penetrating/non-penetrating needles (10 mm insertion depth 31) to each of the 60 subjects at the bilateral TE-5 points, one needle in the right arm and the other in the left, using the alternating twirling technique. After each application, the subjects reported whether they felt a skin penetration sensation and de qi for each arm and wrote down anything that came to their notice. The practitioner guessed the authenticity of the needle after its removal.

Validation test 4 for informed patient masking (28, 30)
We recruited a well-experienced and licensed acupuncturist and 114 healthy volunteers (30.3±7.9 years; 73 men, 41 women). Before the trial, the subjects and the practitioner were informed that penetrating /penetrating, non-penetrating /non-penetrating, and penetrating /non-penetrating needle pairs would be used and that we would ask them about the nature of each needle after it had been removed. We prepared 38 pairs of each of these needle combinations. The insertion depth of the penetrating needle was 5mm (8).
An acupuncturist with eight years experience inserted a pair of needles, randomly taken from the shuffled 114 pairs, at bilateral TE5 points in the subjects and stimulated them using the alternating twirling technique. After each needle application, the subjects guessed the type of the needles. They also rated skin penetration/penetration-like pain and de qi on the VAS ranging from 0 to 10 8, 27. The practitioner was also asked to guess the type of needle after each needle removal (25, 27).

Data Analysis (25, 27, 28)
The chi-squared goodness-of-fit test was used to determine whether the number of correctly and incorrectly identified needles fits a probability of 0.5. Statistical comparisons of practitioner’s confidence scores between correct and incorrect identifications and of the needle groups for VAS scores for skin penetration/penetration-like pain and de qi were made using Mann–Whitney’s U test. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to indicate the relationship between the practitioner’s confidence and skin penetration pain, and between the practitioner’s confidence and de qi. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, version 15.0J (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL). The true identity of the needle was not revealed until after the results had been tabulated.

Results

Validation test 1 for practitioner masking (25, 26)
The number of correct/unidentifiable/incorrect answers given by the 10 acupuncturists had a mean ± SD of 17.0 ± 4.1/6.4 ± 3.6/16.6 ± 3.0, respectively. Overall, the 170 correct and 166 incorrect identifications fitted a probability of 0.5 (chi square = 0.048, p = 0.827), excluding the 64 unidentifiable needles. Furthermore, 107 correctly identified non-penetrating needles and 94 incorrectly identified non-penetrating needles (chi square = 0.841, p = 0.359), and 63 correctly identified penetrating needles and 72 incorrectly identified penetrating needles (chi square = 0.600, p = 0.439) fitted a probability of 0.5 (Table 1).

Validation test 2 for practitioner masking (27)
Of the 60 penetrating needles, 16 (27%) were correctly and 44 (73%) were incorrectly identified by the practitioner; these numbers did not fit a probability of 0.5 (chi square = 13.1, p < 0.001) (Figure 2). On the VAS, the mean confidence of the practitioner in identification of needle authenticity was 50.0 ± 19.7 for correctly identified needles, 73.6 ± 13.7 for incorrectly identified needles, and 67.3 ± 18.6 for all needles. Confidence in incorrect identification was significantly greater than that in correct identification (p < 0.001).

The practitioner guessed the authenticity of the needle principally depending on the “feeling of needle insertion (Table 2).” The second most frequently used determinant was “facial expression.”
Of all the needles that were correctly and incorrectly identified by the practitioner, approximately 90% elicited skin penetration/penetration-like pain and/or de qi in the subjects (Table 3). The skin penetration/penetration-like pain and de qi scores for incorrectly identified needles were not significantly lower than those for correctly identified needles (skin penetration/penetration-like pain, p = 0.09; de qi, p = 0.13) (Figure 3). Further, no significant correlation between the VAS score of practitioner’s confidence in identification and skin penetration/penetration-like pain or between the VAS score and de qi was observed for 16 correctly (skin penetration/penetration-like pain, r = 0.45, p = 0.08; de qi, r = 0.20, p = 0.47) and 44 incorrectly (skin penetration/penetration-like pain, r = 0.24, p = 0.12; de qi, r = 0.08, p = 0.63) identified needles.
Of the 60 needles, 40 (67%) were correctly and 20 (33%) were incorrectly identified by the subjects (Table 4). For the skin penetration/penetration-like pain and de qi scores, there was no significant difference between the 20 needles that were incorrectly and the 40 that were correctly identified (skin penetration/penetration-like pain, p = 0.162; de qi, p = 0.153).

Validation test 3 for uninformed patient masking (25)
None of the subjects commented in the questionnaire that they had received a non-penetrating needle.
Of the 120 needles, the practitioner identified 65 (54.2%) correctly (penetrating needle = 35, non-penetrating needle = 30) and 55 (45.8%) incorrectly (penetrating needle = 25, non-penetrating needle = 30), which fits a probability of 0.5 (2 = 0.833, p = 0.361).
Validation test 4 for informed patient masking 28, 30
Of the total 228 needles applied, the subjects identified 128 (56.1%) needles correctly (non-penetrating = 50, penetrating = 78) and 100 (43.9%) needles incorrectly (non-penetrating = 64, penetrating = 36), fitting the probability of 0.5 (2 = 3.439, p = 0.064).
With regard to the ratings of the skin penetration/penetration-like pain and de qi, no significant differences were found in subjective intensity between the 114 non-penetrating and 114 penetrating needles (Table 5). Of the 114 penetrating needles, 72 (63.2%, 54 correctly and 18 incorrectly identified) elicited skin penetration/penetration-like pain and 40 (35.1%) elicited de qi. Interestingly, 21.1% of the penetrating needles elicited neither response. Of the 114 non-penetrating needles, 72 (63.2%, 20 correctly and 52 incorrectly identified) elicited skin penetration/penetration-like pain and 30 (26.3%) elicited de qi.
The distribution of the non-penetrating needles depicted according to the intensity of skin penetration/penetration-like pain or de qi was similar to that of the penetrating needles (Figure 4). Moreover, the frequencies that needle sensations were elicited in the case of the 114 non-penetrating needles were similar to those in the case of the 114 penetrating needles (Table 6). Furthermore, the frequencies that needle sensations were elicited in the case of non-penetrating needles that were incorrectly and correctly identified were quite similar to those in the case of the penetrating needles that were correctly and incorrectly identified (Table 6).
Of the total 228 needles, the practitioner identified 97 (42.5%) needles correctly (non-penetrating = 54, penetrating = 43) and 122 (53.5%) needles incorrectly (non-penetrating = 54, penetrating = 68), fitting the probability of 0.5 (2 = 2.854, p = 0.091). Nine (4%) needles were indistinguishable.

Discussion

Practitioner masking
The practitioners failed to distinguish between the penetrating and non-penetrating needles, regardless of their practical experience. The appearance and feel of the non-penetrating placebo and penetrating needles in this study were virtually identical, such that the needles caused well-experienced practitioners much difficulty in identifying the true nature of each needle. The fact that the practitioners made incorrect identifications approximately half of the times excluding the unidentifiable needles in the validation study 1, 3 and 4 shows that these needles are useful for masking even with a highly experienced acupuncturist. Further, the highly experienced acupuncturist failed to identify a large proportion of the total number of needles with the finding that a large proportion of needles were identified from the “feeling of needle insertion” in the validation test 2. This indicates that the non-penetrating needle and the penetrating needle were virtually indistinguishable in appearance and feel.

For the incorrectly identified needles in the validation test 2, the subjects’ reactions were not expected to reveal the authenticity of the needle. Seven percent of all needles were identified by “body movement” with “facial expression,” and all of these were correctly identified. However, for these needles, the practitioner’s VAS scores of confidence in identification ranged from 40% to 80%. Thus, although these correctly identified needles were assumed to elicit penetration pain, withdrawal reactions suggesting the authenticity of the needles were obscure. We believe that the penetration pain elicited by an acupuncture needle is relatively weak, the authenticity of needles could not be revealed with 100% confidence. “Facial expression” gave the practitioner little clue about the needle authenticity because approximately two-thirds of needle identification from “facial expression” alone, together with other clues, were incorrect. In addition, the mean VAS score for the practitioner’s confidence in identification of needles identified only from “facial expression” (86% of which were correctly identified) was only 40. Further, there was no significant a positive correlation between the intensity of skin penetration pain or de qi and the degree of the practitioner’s confidence in correct identification of the needle, and a negative correlation for incorrectly identified needles. It is unlikely that the practitioner’s identification was influenced by the subjects’ reactions to the insertion of the needle. Considering that more than 90% of the needles correctly as well as incorrectly identified by the practitioner elicited non-significant different skin penetration/penetration-like pain and de qi, there were very few needles whose authenticity was revealed by the subject’s reactions on needle insertion.
Patient masking
The fully informed subjects failed to correctly identify the non-penetrating needles even though they had previously received acupuncture and were familiar with the different sensations of needle penetration and de qi. The reason for the successful subject masking with the non-penetrating needle was that the sensations elicited by the needle were similar in frequency and intensity to those experienced with needle penetration to a 5mm insertion depth. Thus, subjects who were not informed of the potential use of non-penetrating needles did not suspect that they received a non-penetrating needle in the validation study 3, and the fully informed subjects in the validation study 4 misjudged nearly half of the non-penetrating needles. The difference in skin penetration/penetration-like pain between non-penetrating and penetrating needles was too small to reveal the identity of the non-penetrating needle, as reported in the single-blind study 8. The double-blind placebo needle with a blunt tip is a promising innovation that should allow double-blind acupuncture studies to be undertaken in both non- and fully-informed subjects.

Although we expected it to be unlikely that a subject would misjudge a penetrating needle to be a non-penetrating one, surprisingly, 32% of 114 penetrating needles with 5mm insertion depth in the validation study 4 and 33% of 60 penetrating needles with 10mm insertion depth in the validation study 2 were incorrectly identified by the subjects. The skin penetration pain elicited by these needles was faint and nearly indistinguishable from the sensation elicited by the skin pressure associated with the non-penetrating needle in the validation study 4. Therefore, some penetrating needles were equally misjudged to be non-penetrating when the subjects were informed of the potential application of non-penetrating needles. These incorrect identifications, even though small in proportion, suggest that penetrating needles have some potential for double masking in study using one needle per patient like acupuncture at only the Pericardium 6 point (32).

These results imply that this needle has the potential for in double blinding, but we must be cautious when extrapolating our results because of the following limitations of the study. These studies were not completed in a clinical setting with likely variables such as clinical improvement, adverse reactions and repeat treatments with multiple needles or points, which would risk the practitioner and patient unmasking. Although few bleeding occurred during this series of studies, slight bleeding and patient reaction to strong pain elicited by real needle insertion in a few instances could break the blind. We used only one practitioner in the validation test 2, 3 and 4, so inter-tester reliability should be tested. The subjects were confined to healthy acupuncture students who had previously experience that needle insertion or removal are not necessarily accompanied by pain. We examined only needle insertions to a 5-10 mm depth, using a needle of diameter 0.16 mm, at only two acupoints. The degree of manipulation of the depth or angle of the needle is restricted during needle insertion, although we believe that needle insertion and advancement are the most important components of acupuncture. It is inevitable that any double-blind needle is an artificial device for research that cannot fully reproduce all the conditions of real life acupuncture. It is not known for certain whether this placebo needle has any therapeutic effect or whether the action of acupuncture is point-specific, and these uncertainties should be taken into account when calling this needle placebo and designing studies with it as placebo (26, 34, 35).

Competing interests

N.T. and Hanada College hold US patent 6575992B1, Canadian patent CA 2339223, Korean patent 0478177, Taiwan patent 150135, Chinese patent ZL00800894.9 and Japanese patent 4061397 for the needles described in this article. N.T. is a salaried employee of Hanada College and has received research funding from the college.

Author's contribution

N.T. designed the double-blind needles and the study, performed the data collection and analysis, and wrote the manuscript. H.Y., M.T. and A.K. participated in the study design, the data collection and analysis, and manuscript preparation. N.T. is the guarantor. All authors have read and accepted the final version of the manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We thank Koji Sakurai (Educational Foundation of Hanada Gakuen, Tokyo) and Professor Ikuo Homma (Second Department of Physiology, Showa University School of Medicine, Tokyo) for their support. We also express our appreciation to all participants at Hanada College.

References

1. Bratman S. Double-blind studies A major scientific advance of the 20th century. http://www.mendosa.com/bratman.htm
2. Shapiro AK, Shapiro E. The powerful placebo: From ancient priest to modern physician. London: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 1997:137-174, 190-216.
3. Shapiro AK, Shapiro E. The placebo: Is it much ado about nothing? In Harrington A, ed. The placebo effect: An interdisciplinary exploration. Cambridge, Massachusetts, London, UK: Harvard University Press, 1997:12-36.
4. Kaptchuk TJ. Placebo needle for acupuncture. The Lancet 1998, 352:992.
5. Kaptchuk TJ, Frank E, Shen J, et al. Methodological issues in trials of acupuncture [Letter]. JAMA 2001, 285:1015-1016.
6. Madsen MV, Gøtzsche PC, Hróbjartsson A, Acupuncture treatment for pain: systematic review of randomised clinical trails with acupuncture, placebo acupuncture, and no acupuncture groups. BMJ 2009;338:a3115.
7. Kaptchuk TJ. Acupuncture: theory, efficacy, and practice. Ann Intern Med 2002, 136:374-383.
8. Streitberger K, Kleinhenz J. Introducing a placebo needle into acupuncture research. The Lancet 1998, 352:364-365.
9. Park J, White A, Lee H, Ernst E. Developing of a new sham needle. Acupunct Med 1999; 17:110-2.
10. Park J, White A, Stevinson C, et al. Validating a new non-penetrating sham acupuncture device: Two randomized controlled trials. Acupuncture in Medicine 2002, 20:168-174.
11. Fink M, Gutenbrunner C, Rollnik J, et al. Credibility of a newly designed placebo needle for clinical trials in acupuncture research. Forsch Komplementarmed Klass Naturheilkd 2001, 8:368-372.
12. Melchart D, Streng A, Hoppe A, Brinkhaus B, Witt C, Wagenpfeil S, et al. Acupuncture in patients with tension-type headache: randomized controlled trial. BMJ 2005; 331:376-382.
13. Berman BM, Lao L, Langenberg P, Lee WL, Gilpin AM. Effectiveness of acupuncture as adjunctive therapy in osteoarthritis of the knee: a randomized, controlled trial. Ann Intern Med 2004;141:901-910.
14. Assefi NP, Sherman KJ, Jacobsen C, Goldberg J, Smith WR. A randomized clinical trial of acupuncture compared with sham acupuncture in fibromyalgia. Ann Intern Med 2005; 143:10-19.
15. Witt C, Brinkhaus B, Jena S, Linde K, Streng A, Wagenpfeil S et al. Acupuncture in patients with osteoarthritis of the knee: a randomized trial. The Lancet 2005; 366:136-143.
16. Streitberger K, Diefenbacher M, Bauer A, Conradi R, Bardenheuer H, Martin E et al. Acupuncture compared to placebo-acupuncture for postoperative nausea and vomiting prophylaxis: a randomized placebo-controlled patient and observer blind trial. Anaesthesia 2004; 59:142-149.
17. Shen J, Wenger N, Glaspy J, Hays RD, Albert PS, Choi C et al. Electroacupuncture for control of myeloablative chemotherapy-induced emesis. A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2000; 284:2755-2761.
18. Kaptchuk TJ: The placebo effect in alternative medicine: can the performance of a healing ritual have clinical significance? Ann Intern Med 2002, 136: 817-825.
19. Gracely RH, Dubener R, Deeter WR, Wolskee PJ: Clinicians’ expectations influence placebo analgesia. The Lancet 1985, 15: 43.
20. Turner JA, Deyo RA, Losser JD, Korff MV, Fordyce WE: The importance of placebo effects in pain treatment and research. JAMA 1994, 271:1609-1614.
21. Kenneth FS, Iain C, Richard JH, Douglas GA: Empirical evidence of bias. Dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA 1995, 273: 408-412.
22. Takakura N, Yajima H. Is only patient masking acceptable for acupuncture studies? http://bmj.com/cgi/eletters/335/7617/436#178586, 22 Oct 2007
23. Takakura N, Yajima H. Placebo needle http://bmj.com/cgi/eletters/336/7651/999#196673, 5 Jun 2008
24. Takakura N, Hanada College: Safe needle, placebo needle, and needle set for double blind. US patent 6575992B1 2003 June 10, PCT/JP00/03144. 2000 May.
25. Takakura N, Yajima H: A double–blind placebo needle for acupuncture research. BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine 2007, 7:31.
26. White A. New sham acupuncture needle. Acupunct Med 2008; 26(1):57.
27. Takakura N, Takayama M, Kawase A, Yajima H. Double-blind acupuncture needling: Does patient reaction reveal needle authenticity? Medical Acupuncture 2008; 20: 169-174.
28. Takakura N, Yajima H. A placebo acupuncture needle with potential for double blinding- a validation study. Acupunct Med 2008; 26(4):224-230.
29. Takakura N, Yajima H. A possible double-blind acupuncture placebo needle
http://bmj.com/cgi/eletters/338/jan27_2/a3115#208302, 5 Feb 2009
30. Takakura N, Yajima H, Foster NE. Validation study of a placebo acupuncture needle. Fact 2009;14(2):117-119.
31. Pomeranz B, Berman B: Scientific basis of acupuncture. In: Basics of Acupuncture. Edited by Stux G, Berman B, Pomeranz B. 5th Edition, Berlin Heidelberg New York, Springer-Verlag 2003, 146-148, 202, 51-55.
32. Smith C. Crowther C, Beilby J. Acupuncture to treat nausea and vomiting in early pregnancy: a randomized controlled trial. Birth. 2002; 29: 1-9.
33. White P, Lewith G, Hopwood V, Prescott P. The placebo needle, is it a valid and convincing placebo for use in acupucture trials? A randomized, single-blind, cross-over pilot trial. Pain 2003; 106: 401-409.
34. L Lao, J Ezzo, B M Berman, R Hammerschlag. Assessing clinical efficacy of acupuncture: considerations for designing future acupuncture trials. In Clinical Acupuncture Scientific Basis. Stux G, Hammerschlag, Edited. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer-Verlag; 2001; p194.
35. Kaptchuk TJ, Stason WB, Davis RB, Legedza ATR, Schnyer RN, Kerr CE, et al. Sham device v inert pill: randomized controlled trial of two placebo treatments. BMJ 2006; 332: 391-7.

鍼灸師・医師・研究者・
学生・賛同される方へ

入会のご案内 学術大会・
研修会
学会認定